You'll probably remember exactly where you were and what you were doing when you first learned that passenger jets had crashed into the World Trade Center. People tend to form exceptionally vivid memories of highly consequential news, and it doesn't get much bigger than 9/11.
Recollections of that day have given
researchers a unique window into how the brain forms memories of
shocking events. "It's as if a flashbulb goes off and you take a mental
picture of your surroundings," says psychologist William Hirst of the New York School for Social Research.
Flashbulb memories,
as they are known, are tricky to study as people are seldom keen to
talk to researchers just after hearing or seeing emotionally charged
news. It can also be difficult to know how accurate a person's memory of
the event is, since there is usually no way to be sure what actually
happened.
Elizabeth Phelps
of New York University was in Manhattan on 9/11 and saw the attack.
When fellow neuroscientist John Gabrieli called to check on her they
"decided to put together a consortium of memory researchers, and started
collecting data within a week".
Public tragedy
"It struck us that 9/11 was probably
one of the best examples of a public tragedy that we could use [to study
flashbulb memory]," says Hirst.
The study involved surveying over 3000
Americans from seven different cities, including New York, within two
to three days of the attack. Participants were asked to note all the
details they could remember of the day itself, their personal
circumstances at the time, and how they felt. To find out whether the
memories formed would be lasting ones, the group sent out the same
survey to the same participants again 11 months later, and once more 35
months after the attacks.
"People were about 60 per cent right
about the details of the event after about a year, and this dropped to
50 per cent after three years," suggesting flashbulb memories are no
more accurate than other types of memory, says Phelps. But that didn't
affect how vividly people recalled the day, or how much faith they had
in the details of their memories (Journal of Experimental Psychology, DOI: 10.1037/a0015527).
But evidence that flashbulb memories might be processed differently to other types of memory was provided by Patrick Davidson's
team at the University of Arizona in Tucson, who investigated whether
memories for 9/11 degraded in the same way as other memories in older
individuals. The group found that while older people with frontal lobe
damage struggled to remember other personal events, they remembered 9/11
as well as young people, suggesting that flashbulb memories might be
uniquely impervious to ageing.
Malleable memories
These vivid yet inaccurate memories
might well be the brain's best way of dealing with an emotional,
life-changing event, says Phelps. Keeping the memory malleable to some
extent can have benefits: "If a group arrives at a memory of shared
suffering, it could be a positive thing in terms of group spirit and
identity, and could pull the community together," says Hirst.
When Phelps reassessed the survey
responses recently, she found that people's memories of where they were
on 9/11 have stayed consistent, though how they felt is harder to
recall. "People tended to have around 80 per cent accuracy in their
place memories, but were only 40 per cent accurate at remembering their
emotions," says Phelps, who is writing up the finding. A weak emotional
memory could also be useful in getting over tragic events.
In another study three years after the
attacks, Phelps used functional MRI to monitor the brain activity of
New Yorkers as they recalled the events of 9/11, along with other
personal, life-changing events from the same period. She found that when
recalling 9/11 those who had been within 4 kilometres of the Twin
Towers during the attack had relatively higher activity in the amygdala –
a region of the brain involved in emotion – than those who had been
further away, suggesting that flashbulb memories may be encoded
differently in the brains of those closest to the attack (Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0609230103).
This theory has been backed by surveys by Olivier Luminet
and Antonietta Curci, at the Catholic University of Louvain in Belgium.
The pair found that, while both US and non-US citizens developed
flashbulb memories of the event, they appeared to have done so in
different ways. US citizens who had rated the attack as more surprising
had more consistent memories, but non-US citizens were better able to
remember the event when they were more emotionally affected by it (Memory, DOI: 10.1080/09658210903081827).
A decade on, can you be sure what you
were doing when you heard of the attacks? Phelps and Hirst are
conducting a 10-year follow-up of their original participants that will,
for the first time, show how flashbulb memories survive in the long
term.
background keka.. and matter variety..
ReplyDelete